際際滷

際際滷Share a Scribd company logo
The Influence of Essay Prompt Directedness
on the Content Quality of Summary Writing
by
Roy B. Clariana (rbc4@psu.edu), College of Education
Ryan Solnosky (rls5008@psu.edu), College of Engineering
The Pennsylvania State University
A presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA),
Chicago, IL, April 14, 2023. Funding from the Division of Undergraduate Education of the National
Science Foundation (Award Abstract #2215807), Roy B. Clariana (PI). link
The Influence of Essay Prompt Directedness
on the Content Quality of Summary Writing
 As an important step in the development of GIKS browser-based writing-to-learn software
that provides immediate structural feedback, we seek ways to improve the quality of
students essays and to optimize the software analysis algorithm.
 This quasi-experimental investigation compares the quality of students summary writing
under three writing prompt conditions, otherwise identical prompts add either 0, 14, or 26
key terms.
 Results show that key terms matter substantially  the summary essays of those given
the prompt without key terms had longer essays and the resulting networks of those
essays were more like the expert referent and like their peers essays.
 Although tentative, these results indicate that writing prompts should NOT include key
terms.
2 of 10
GIKS browser-based tool (new version)
3 of 10
You can play with GIKS at: https://tinyurl.com/MyGIKs
4 of 10
Network feedback screen
Participants
Participating students in two separate sections of the 3-credit residential course
AE 222 Building Modeling and Documentation were asked to complete one of
three similar essay prompts, called Focus, Full, and no terms
 Focus group n = 44
 Full group, n = 40
 No terms group = 73
Note: this quasi-experimental design adds a control no terms condition
Focus  the central nodes in the expert network
Full  central + peripheral nodes in the expert network
5 of 10
Dissertation by Yu Wang (2022), randomly assigned to groups
Sample of convenience one year later
See next slide for details
Materials
Essay prompt:
Reflect back on the readings and lecture this
week regarding wood-frame building. Write a
broad description of this in your own words
(about 300 words long). Here are some key
terms that you could include in your
summary: fastener, floor, framing, joists, nails,
opening, plate, platform, roof truss,
residential, roof, sheathing, stud, walls,
blocking, bottom plate, commercial, door, fire
stop, floor truss, header, rafter, subfloor, top
plate, window, wood
6 of 10
In the question (2)
Focus terms (14)
Full terms (14 +12)
Instructors network
Focus
Full
General Findings
 Essay length  Open > Focus (p < .001, ES = 1.21), and Open > Full (p < .001, ES = 1.06),
Focus fell midway between the two, adding key terms to the writing prompt decreased
the total number of words in the essays.
 Central terms  including key terms (full, focus, none) in the writing prompt made no
difference on the number of central terms in the essays. (see term freq. slide 9)
 Peripheral terms  Including key terms (full, focus, none) in the writing prompt made no
difference on the number of peripheral terms in the essays. (see term freq. slide 9)
 Essay network similarity with the expert network as a measure of essay quality 
Counterintuitively, including only the central key terms in the writing prompt decreased
the essay network similarity to the expert. Open > Focus (p = .001, ES = .61), Full fell
between the two
 Essay network similarity with peers averaged network as a measure of peer-peer
knowledge convergence  Open > Focus (p = .003, ES = .64), Full fell between the two
7 of 10
NSD
Findings
8 of 10
Essay length  Open > Focus (p < .001, ES = 1.21), and Open > Full (p <
.001, ES = 1.06), Focus fell midway between the two, adding key terms to
the writing prompt decreased the total number of words in the essays.
Essay network similarity with the expert network as a measure of essay
quality  Counterintuitively, including only the central key terms in the
writing prompt decreased the essay network similarity to the expert.
Open > Focus (p = .001, ES = .61), Full fell between the two
Essay network similarity with peers averaged network as a measure of
peer-peer knowledge convergence  Open > Focus (p = .003, ES = .64),
Full fell between the two
In this case, adding terms to the essay
prompt messed up the essay networks
structure relative to the expert network
and to peers essay networks
9 of 10
0.00
1.00
2.00
3.00
4.00
5.00
6.00
7.00
8.00
9.00
10.00
student essay term freq.
H focus H full H none
Term
average
frequency
Full group (solid)
Focus (dashed)
Prompt (3) + 14 focus terms 12 full terms Remaining 21 high frequency terms
no terms group (dashed)
framing
wood
floors
walls
roof
building
studs
joists
sheathing
platform
residential
plates
nails
opening
roof
truss
fastener
timber
construction
foundation
top
plate
rafter
commercial
subfloor
window
fire
header
bottom
plate
door
blocking
floor
truss
type
member
light
support
wood
framing
balloon
structure
floor
joist
step
exterior
light
wood
material
structural
concrete
platform
framing
interior
wall
framing
roof
framing
typically
truss
component
2
 Central terms  including key terms in the writing prompt (full, focus, none)
made no difference on the number of central terms in the essays.
 Peripheral terms  Including key terms in the writing prompt (full, focus,
none) made no difference on the number of peripheral terms in the essays.
You would expect that the Focus group essays would have
higher term frequency for the focus terms
and
the Full group essays would have higher term frequency for
the Full terms
This did NOT occur, term frequency in essays was
consistent across all treatments (full, focus, none)
Term frequencies
Discussion/Conclusion
 Reason and common sense would suggest that including key terms in a writing prompt should scaffold
writing and would thus result in better essays. For example, low-directed concept mapping, where no key
terms are provided, tended to better reveal conceptual understanding (both explanations and errors) than
did high-directed mapping that provided some or all terms and some structure (Ruiz-Primo et al. 2001).
This did NOT happen, no terms was best. Why?
 Pre-existing group differences are the most likely explanation of the findings (quasi-experimental design)
 But perhaps including key terms in the writing prompt disrupted retrieval during writing in this case because
the alphabetical list of terms misaligns with both the writers conceptual structure of this content as well as
with the actual lesson conceptual structure. Research into part-list cuing inhibition (Slamecka, 1968) has
shown that providing several items as cues counterintuitively decreases recall memory relative to no cues.
Basden, Basden, and Stephens (2002) explain this inhibition by showing that cue order maters. Inhibition
could occur when the structure of the cue (i.e., here, the alphabetical list of key terms in the prompt)
mismatches the undergirding conceptual structure of the lesson.
 Alternately, terms lists in the prompt may influence regulation regarding, is my essay complete now?, i..e,
once you include all the terms, you are done
 Further research is needed to determine if key terms inhibition observed in this investigation, as well as part-
list cuing inhibition, are mediated by matching or mismatching of conceptual structures of the learner, the
cue, and the lesson.
10 of 10
Questions?

More Related Content

Clariana AERA 2023 presentation.pptx

  • 1. The Influence of Essay Prompt Directedness on the Content Quality of Summary Writing by Roy B. Clariana (rbc4@psu.edu), College of Education Ryan Solnosky (rls5008@psu.edu), College of Engineering The Pennsylvania State University A presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), Chicago, IL, April 14, 2023. Funding from the Division of Undergraduate Education of the National Science Foundation (Award Abstract #2215807), Roy B. Clariana (PI). link
  • 2. The Influence of Essay Prompt Directedness on the Content Quality of Summary Writing As an important step in the development of GIKS browser-based writing-to-learn software that provides immediate structural feedback, we seek ways to improve the quality of students essays and to optimize the software analysis algorithm. This quasi-experimental investigation compares the quality of students summary writing under three writing prompt conditions, otherwise identical prompts add either 0, 14, or 26 key terms. Results show that key terms matter substantially the summary essays of those given the prompt without key terms had longer essays and the resulting networks of those essays were more like the expert referent and like their peers essays. Although tentative, these results indicate that writing prompts should NOT include key terms. 2 of 10
  • 3. GIKS browser-based tool (new version) 3 of 10 You can play with GIKS at: https://tinyurl.com/MyGIKs
  • 4. 4 of 10 Network feedback screen
  • 5. Participants Participating students in two separate sections of the 3-credit residential course AE 222 Building Modeling and Documentation were asked to complete one of three similar essay prompts, called Focus, Full, and no terms Focus group n = 44 Full group, n = 40 No terms group = 73 Note: this quasi-experimental design adds a control no terms condition Focus the central nodes in the expert network Full central + peripheral nodes in the expert network 5 of 10 Dissertation by Yu Wang (2022), randomly assigned to groups Sample of convenience one year later See next slide for details
  • 6. Materials Essay prompt: Reflect back on the readings and lecture this week regarding wood-frame building. Write a broad description of this in your own words (about 300 words long). Here are some key terms that you could include in your summary: fastener, floor, framing, joists, nails, opening, plate, platform, roof truss, residential, roof, sheathing, stud, walls, blocking, bottom plate, commercial, door, fire stop, floor truss, header, rafter, subfloor, top plate, window, wood 6 of 10 In the question (2) Focus terms (14) Full terms (14 +12) Instructors network Focus Full
  • 7. General Findings Essay length Open > Focus (p < .001, ES = 1.21), and Open > Full (p < .001, ES = 1.06), Focus fell midway between the two, adding key terms to the writing prompt decreased the total number of words in the essays. Central terms including key terms (full, focus, none) in the writing prompt made no difference on the number of central terms in the essays. (see term freq. slide 9) Peripheral terms Including key terms (full, focus, none) in the writing prompt made no difference on the number of peripheral terms in the essays. (see term freq. slide 9) Essay network similarity with the expert network as a measure of essay quality Counterintuitively, including only the central key terms in the writing prompt decreased the essay network similarity to the expert. Open > Focus (p = .001, ES = .61), Full fell between the two Essay network similarity with peers averaged network as a measure of peer-peer knowledge convergence Open > Focus (p = .003, ES = .64), Full fell between the two 7 of 10 NSD
  • 8. Findings 8 of 10 Essay length Open > Focus (p < .001, ES = 1.21), and Open > Full (p < .001, ES = 1.06), Focus fell midway between the two, adding key terms to the writing prompt decreased the total number of words in the essays. Essay network similarity with the expert network as a measure of essay quality Counterintuitively, including only the central key terms in the writing prompt decreased the essay network similarity to the expert. Open > Focus (p = .001, ES = .61), Full fell between the two Essay network similarity with peers averaged network as a measure of peer-peer knowledge convergence Open > Focus (p = .003, ES = .64), Full fell between the two In this case, adding terms to the essay prompt messed up the essay networks structure relative to the expert network and to peers essay networks
  • 9. 9 of 10 0.00 1.00 2.00 3.00 4.00 5.00 6.00 7.00 8.00 9.00 10.00 student essay term freq. H focus H full H none Term average frequency Full group (solid) Focus (dashed) Prompt (3) + 14 focus terms 12 full terms Remaining 21 high frequency terms no terms group (dashed) framing wood floors walls roof building studs joists sheathing platform residential plates nails opening roof truss fastener timber construction foundation top plate rafter commercial subfloor window fire header bottom plate door blocking floor truss type member light support wood framing balloon structure floor joist step exterior light wood material structural concrete platform framing interior wall framing roof framing typically truss component 2 Central terms including key terms in the writing prompt (full, focus, none) made no difference on the number of central terms in the essays. Peripheral terms Including key terms in the writing prompt (full, focus, none) made no difference on the number of peripheral terms in the essays. You would expect that the Focus group essays would have higher term frequency for the focus terms and the Full group essays would have higher term frequency for the Full terms This did NOT occur, term frequency in essays was consistent across all treatments (full, focus, none) Term frequencies
  • 10. Discussion/Conclusion Reason and common sense would suggest that including key terms in a writing prompt should scaffold writing and would thus result in better essays. For example, low-directed concept mapping, where no key terms are provided, tended to better reveal conceptual understanding (both explanations and errors) than did high-directed mapping that provided some or all terms and some structure (Ruiz-Primo et al. 2001). This did NOT happen, no terms was best. Why? Pre-existing group differences are the most likely explanation of the findings (quasi-experimental design) But perhaps including key terms in the writing prompt disrupted retrieval during writing in this case because the alphabetical list of terms misaligns with both the writers conceptual structure of this content as well as with the actual lesson conceptual structure. Research into part-list cuing inhibition (Slamecka, 1968) has shown that providing several items as cues counterintuitively decreases recall memory relative to no cues. Basden, Basden, and Stephens (2002) explain this inhibition by showing that cue order maters. Inhibition could occur when the structure of the cue (i.e., here, the alphabetical list of key terms in the prompt) mismatches the undergirding conceptual structure of the lesson. Alternately, terms lists in the prompt may influence regulation regarding, is my essay complete now?, i..e, once you include all the terms, you are done Further research is needed to determine if key terms inhibition observed in this investigation, as well as part- list cuing inhibition, are mediated by matching or mismatching of conceptual structures of the learner, the cue, and the lesson. 10 of 10