1) Evolutionary psychology suggests that humans are more likely to help those who are genetically related due to kin selection, as helping relatives increases the likelihood of passing on shared genes.
2) Research has found that people are more inclined to help those closely related to them, such as young children and close family members, compared to distant relatives or non-relatives, especially in life-threatening situations. Females and younger individuals also tend to receive more help.
3) The bystander effect occurs when the presence of others discourages an individual from helping in an emergency situation due to diffusion of responsibility; people are less likely to help as the number of bystanders increases.
2. Definitions
Prosocial behavior = any act performed
with the goal of benefiting another person
Altruism = motive/desire to help another
person even if it involves a cost (or at least
no benefit) to the helper
no rewards for helping
3. Evolutionary psychology
Who would you save?
Twin vs. mother/father
save the person who is closer in our genetics, to pass
on your genetics
4. Evolutionary psychology
1 vs. 5 year old child
save the 5 year old, they will be more likely to survive
20 vs. 40 year old child
save 20 year old, in reproductive stage
5. Evolutionary psychology
Who is the kinder grandmother?
maternal grandmother, because of maternal certainty
(not sure with paternal grandparents, that it is the
males child)
7. Why do we help?
Evolutionary Psychology
If the goal is to ensure our own survival, why should
we help others at a cost to ourselves?
Kin selection = behaviors that help a genetic relative
are favored by natural selection
Thus, a gene that causes an individual to help
genetic relatives is actually helping a copy of itself
Example: People say they would be more likely to
help their relatives than their non-relatives in life-
threatening situations
8. Who do we help?
Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994)
Participants in this study were asked to imagine
scenarios like the following:
There are three people asleep in different rooms of
a burning house:
Your 7 year-old female cousin
Your 75 year-old grandfather
A 21 year-old acquaintance
You have time to rescue only one
Who do you save?
11. Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994):
Findings
Kin are helped more than non-kin, especially in life-
or-death situations
Females are helped more than males, except elderly
females (post-menopausal)
Young are helped more than old
Healthy relatives helped more than non-healthy in
life-or-death situations
In life-or-death helping, relatedness matters (this
assures that our genes will continue)
In everyday helping situations, needs prevail over
genes
12. Why do we help?
Evolutionary Psychology
Why do we help non-kin?
Reciprocity: helping others now ensures
that they help us later
Adaptive strategy for our ancestors
becomes genetically-based tendency
13. Why do we help?
Evolutionary psychology
Learning social norms
People who learn norms have a survival
advantage ability to learn norms becomes
genetically-based
Other social norms
Social-responsibility norm = we should help
those who are dependent on us
Norm of justice = we should help those who
deserve help
14. Why do we help?
Social Learning Theory
Helping is learned through observation and
reinforcement
Children learn to help by being rewarded
As people mature, reinforcements become
less necessary
internalize the value of helping
15. Why do we help?
Social Exchange Theory
Maximizing rewards and minimizing costs
People will help when the rewards are high relative
to the costs
Rewards: social approval, feeling good about
yourself, increasing likelihood of being helped in
future
Costs: physical danger, time, embarrassment, guilt
16. Who will help?
Gender Differences
Women are more likely to give long-term,
nurturing help
Men are more likely to help in emergencies,
especially when there is:
an audience
potential danger
a woman in need of help
17. Who will help?
Religiosity
Religious people are only slightly more
likely to help during emergencies
Religious people are more likely to
provide planned help
Examples:
volunteering, giving to charity
18. Who will help?
Mood
good moods can lead to helpful behavior
Examples
Tips on sunny vs. cloudy days
Isen and Levin (1972)
IV: found a dime in coin return slot of telephone
DV: Help confederate pick up papers
no dime -> 4% helped
dime -> 84% helped
19. Who will help?
Why do good moods predict pro-social
behavior?
Helping maintains good mood
Good moods make us see the good in people
positive thoughts -> positive behavior
Good moods increase self-awareness
More likely to act in accordance with our values
20. Who will help?
Mood
Bad moods can sometimes lead to prosocial
behavior
Negative-state relief hypothesis: people help to
alleviate their own bad mood
Exceptions: people who are very depressed or
angry do not tend to help much
21. Mood
Guilt: Feelings of guilt tend to increase the
likelihood of helping
Churchgoers are more likely to contribute
to a charity before confession than
afterward
Breaking a camera increases likelihood
of helping a completely different person
22. Similarity
we are more likely to help those similar to us
We like those who are similar to us
Liking lecture and shared b-day study
Example: Students in England who identified them
selves as fans of the Manchester United soccer football
team were assigned to see another student fall and act
as though they were in pain (Levine et al., 2005).
This student was wearing a Manchester United or
rival Liverpool shirt
results: students were almost 4 times more likely to
help the student in the United shirt
23. Situational Influences
Time pressure
Good Samaritan study
Princeton Theological Seminary students were
told they were either early or late to give a talk
They all encountered a man slumped in a
doorway who was coughing and groaning.
How many people stopped to see if the man
needed help?
24. Good Samaritans
Results
Had time/early: 65% helped
Running late: 10% helped
Topic of talk had no effect on helping!
25. Situational Influences
Rural vs. urban environment
Example: staged injury
Small town: about 50% of the pedestrians
offered to help
Large city: about 15% of pedestrians offered to
help
Why the difference?
26. Situational Influences
Why do people help more in small towns?
Urban overload hypothesis = people
living in cities are constantly bombarded
with stimulation, so they keep to
themselves to avoid being overwhelmed
immediate surroundings matter more than
internalized values
27. Latane and Darley (1970)
Students in cubicles communicating over
intercom (alone, 1, or 4 students)
one student has a seizure
28. Bystander Effect
100 Alone
1 other bystander
4 other bystanders
75
Percent 50
who helped
25
0
60 seconds 150 seconds
Number of seconds elapsed from
start of seizure
29. Bystander Effect
the more
bystanders who
witness an
emergency, the
less likely one of
them will help
30. Bystander Intervention
Step 1:
What prevents Step 1?
Distraction: other people distract our attention
Manners: we dont stare at others; we keep
our eyes to ourselves
31. Step 1
Example: Smoke-filled room study
Participants filled out a questionnaire either
alone or with two strangers
Staged emergency: smoke poured into the
room through a wall vent
Who noticed the smoke more quickly?
Participants working alone noticed the smoke
almost immediately
Participants working in small groups took longer
32. Bystander Intervention
Step 2: Interpreting the event as an
emergency
What prevents Step 2?
ambiguity
pluralistic ignorance
33. Step 2
Interpreting the event as an emergency
Pluralistic ignorance: The state in which people
mistakenly believe that their own thoughts and
feelings are different from those of others, even
though everyones behavior is the same
specifically, bystanders assume nothing is wrong
in an emergency becuse no one else looks
concerned
Smoke-filled room study
34. Step 2
Example: Smoke-filled room study
Most people in groups continued to work on the
questionnaire as they coughed and waved smoke away
with their hands.
People glanced at others (saw that they were working
diligently)
Participants interpretation of the smoke:
A leak in the air condition
Steam pipes
Chemistry labs in the building
Truth gas
35. Bystander Intervention
Step 3: Assuming responsibility
What prevents Step 3?
Diffusion of responsibility = each bystanders
sense of responsibility to help decreases as the
number of bystanders increases
When people are alone, they feel
responsible.
When people are not alone, everyone places
the responsibility on everyone else.
36. Step 4: Decide how to help
Why do people not help? They do not
want to appear foolish
prevents = lack of knowledge and
competence
37. Step 4: Decide how to help
Cramer et al. (1988)
Emergency (seizure)
Participants were students or nurses
Participants alone or with others
38. Step 4: Decide how to help
Results
Alone or with others, 70-75% of
nurses helped
70% of students helped when alone;
25% helped when with others
39. Bystander Intervention
Step 5: Deciding to implement the help
What prevents Step 5?
Costs of helping
legal problems
embarrassment (audience inhibition)
personal danger
40. Step 5: Must actually provide help
Audience inhibition
fear that others will evaluate them
negatively if they intervene and the
situation is not an emergency
41. How can helping be increased?
Increase the likelihood that bystanders will intervene
Reduce ambiguity
Increase responsibility
Increase self-awareness
give specific instructions
teach people about the bystander effect